Language and Literacy
Jeanne S. Chall was born in Poland on January 1, 1921. She moved to New York at a tender age of seven with her family. Jeanne S. Chall was one of the chief educators and researchers in the field of literacy during the past century. The Harvard Reading/Literacy Lab has recently been renamed in accolade of Dr. Chall.
What follows is an account of Dr. Chall's life and work. Chall grew up in New York City, taught there, and received her bachelor's degree from City College in 1941. Due to a dearth of teaching posts open during the early 1940's, Chall took an assistantship at Teacher's College, Columbia University, subordinate to Irving Lorge, an intelligence-test researcher. It was there at Teacher's College that Chall first advanced a fascination and liking for educational research.
Chall then went on to seek her master's and doctoral degrees at Ohio State University under the teaching of Edgar Dale. Chall was a fellow of the American Psychological Association, a member of the National Academy of Education and the Reading Hall of Fame, and had worked on the Board of Directors of the International Reading Association and the National Society for the Study of Education. She has received numerous awards, including the Edward L. Thorndike award from the American Psychological Association for remarkable psychological offering to education, the American Educational Research Association's award for notable contributions to educational research, and the Samuel T. Orton Award from the Orton Dyslexia Society. She is also a member of Ohio State University College of Education Hall of Fame. Jeanne S. Chall was called upon by a succession of U.S. presidents and secretaries of education to bring her understanding to national literacy efforts. She was an advisor to projects organized by the Office of Education, the National Institute of Education, the National Academy of Education, and many others. She made herself accessible and helpful to the many students, teachers, and researchers who liked her advice and wise instructions.
Though Chall retired from teaching in 1991, she still continued to be a principal contributor to the field of Reading Research as a writer, advisor, and mentor until she passed away in the fall of 1999. Fortunately, Chall served as an inspiration to some of the most prominent researchers and practitioners currently working to improve literacy instruction. Benita Blachman, Steve Stahl, and Maryanne Wolfe are just a few of Chall's most well-known successors.
Accomplishments of Jeanne S. Chall:
Jeanne Chall has had two dominant roles in the field of reading. She has been an earnest researcher and has been the inspiration behind the work of others. Literacy instruction, classroom organization and literacy assessment were the principles that guided her work.
In this remarkable volume...Jeanne Chall made a tremendous contribution to American education, a contribution that could revolutionize the way we approach teaching and learning....Any school board really concerned about how to improve student learning should consider buying this book in volume...to make certain it reaches all who direct instruction....It is a highly readable book, numbering under 200 pages, but each page is chock-full of information, provocative questions, and ideas that should stir the heart of anyone from a policy worker to a classroom teacher in P.S. 100 trying to do his or her best to teach children. Chall's mastery of the past century and more of research on the issue of what works is truly remarkable.... Public education is under almost constant attack from one quarter or another. The 2000 presidential campaign promises to elevate the issue to epic proportions. Rather than resort to a score of new programs, a thousand new ideas, perhaps we should simply require that every candidate, local, state or national, read this treasure of a book before they engage in debate. Then we might actually focus our energy and resources where it belongs - on improving teaching and learning."
Jeanne S. Chall was the most concentrated and single-minded individual in the literary circles of United States. She was beyond doubt dedicated to assisting every child in the U.S. get to read not only fluently but for the true joy of it as well. Her great book, 'Learning to Read: The Great Debate', published in 1967, was the first to enunciate clearly the essential elements of reading.
The greatest hallmark of the work of Jeanne S. Chall has been her consideration for poor children and the necessity to guarantee that they receive the same teaching and access to literary challenges that more rich children have. She spent several years unraveling the link between early reading and later academic achievement. In her 1983 book, 'Stages of Reading Development' Chall characterized how readers advance from maturing elementary skills through the most complex reading, arguing that unless students are fluent in decoding...
Sometimes students have obstacles to contend with as they enter school. One such barrier can be language. The student I worked with is a Chinese first year student who is attempting to assimilate to AP class schedules. He is a 14-year old interested in learning the English language and is having problems not only learning the language but balancing out the needs of his identity versus the American culture. English
Devel Language skills are fundamental to child social and psychological development, because language provides the means by which children learn about the world and other people. Parents impart language skills cues to their children by teaching coded and decoded messages. Some parental messages are encoded subtly, as with emotional responses. For example, Paulson, Keefe, & Leiferman (2009) found that parental depression impacts the reading habits of parents, which in turn
Language and Thinking Language is the one aspect, which distinguishes human beings from lower species of life (Faccone et al. 2000). Sternberg (1999 as qtd in Faccone et al.) lists its properties as including communication, arbitrary symbolism, regular structure, structure at multiple levels, generation and production and dynamism. Sternberg assumes that language is most likely acquired naturally from the environment where a person is raised as an infant. The stages seem
One piece of evidence that suggests there is at least some degree of "hardwiring" of language in the human brain is the fact that very similar mistakes are made in certain grammatical forms and syntax structures by early speakers of any language. There seems to be an innate sense of the way words are supposed to be formed and fit together, and instances that do not follow these expected/innate rules
Language and Social Grouping Language is used differently in different geographic groups, ethnic, age, gender, and socioeconomic groups (Williams, 2010). Geographic groups use the same languages in different dialects that belong to the particular geographic regions. Within each language are many different dialects that have been formed with different geographic locations and cultures. Shared words, experiences, cultures, and expressions are ethnic and shared elements of the social fabric. Language of a common
These activities help children to learn the difference between contextualized and decontextualized language. "When we write, read, and have conversations, we often use decontextualized language. This is language that is not tied to the immediate context. It may reflect past events, future events, or fictitious events. For example, decontextualized language is used in everyday dinnertime conversation, when adults tell stories of their childhood, or when children tell about their
Our semester plans gives you unlimited, unrestricted access to our entire library of resources —writing tools, guides, example essays, tutorials, class notes, and more.
Get Started Now