Verified Document

Extrinsic Motivation Essay

Extrinsic motivation refers to behavior that is driven by external rewards such as money, fame, grades, and praise. This type of motivation arises from outside the individual, as opposed to intrinsic motivation, which involves engaging in a behavior because it is personally rewarding; essentially, performing an activity for its own sake rather than the desire for some external reward (Ryan & Deci, 2000).

One of the primary theories associated with extrinsic motivation is the behaviorist concept of operant conditioning. B.F. Skinner, a well-known psychologist, suggested that behavior is a function of its consequences; thus, behavior that is reinforced tends to be repeated (i.e., strengthened), while behavior that is not reinforced tends to die out-or be extinguished (i.e., weakened) (Skinner, 1953). For example, when a student receives a reward (such as a gold star) for a high test score, this positive reinforcement may increase the likelihood that the student will study for future tests.

In the context of the workplace, extrinsic motivation is often used to motivate employees through monetary bonuses, promotions, status symbols like corner offices, or the threat of job loss (Pink, 2009). The underlying assumption is that employees will increase productivity or improve performance to obtain these external rewards or to avoid negative consequences such as demotion or dismissal.

However, it is important to consider the potential downsides of heavy reliance on extrinsic motivation. Research has shown that while extrinsic rewards can be effective at motivating certain types of task performance in the short term, they can sometimes lead to a decrease in intrinsic motivation (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999). This could manifest as reduced engagement or decreased creativity when the rewards are not available, reflecting a shift in focus from the enjoyment and satisfaction derived from the task itself to the pursuit of the rewards associated with the task.

Educational settings often serve as a natural laboratory for the exploration of extrinsic motivation. Grades, diplomas, and scholarships are common extrinsic motivators for students. The expectation of receiving these rewards can lead students to engage in behaviors such as cheating or rote memorization rather than truly engaging with the material and developing a deeper understanding (Kohn, 1993). This calls into question the effectiveness of extrinsic rewards in fostering genuine learning and underscores the need for a balance between intrinsic and extrinsic motivators.

The Self-Determination Theory (SDT) further explains the interplay between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. According to Ryan and Deci (2000), external motivations can be internalized and turned into personally endorsed values, if they are presented in a way that supports an individual's sense of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. For instance, when students internalize the importance of learning for their future careers, the previously extrinsic motivation of grades may be transformed into an intrinsic desire to learn and grow.

Delving further into the nuances of extrinsic motivation, research has shown that the effectiveness of extrinsic rewards can be contingent on the perceived autonomy of the individual (Deci & Ryan, 1987). When extrinsic rewards are perceived as controlling or as undermining one's sense of self-determination, they are more likely to have a detrimental effect on intrinsic motivation. However, if rewards are perceived as informational and affirming competence without constraining autonomy, they may be less likely to have negative effects on intrinsic motivation and could even enhance it under certain circumstances (Ryan, 1982).

In the context of performance-contingent rewards, which are given based on the level of performance attained, there can be complex effects on motivation. On one hand, these rewards can provide clear standards and goals that may enhance certain types of task engagement. On the other hand, they can lead to an increased pressure to perform, which may undermine intrinsic motivation or lead to anxiety, especially if the rewards are seen as too challenging or unattainable (Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996).

Self-Perception Theory also provides insight into how external rewards might affect intrinsic motivation. Daryl Bem (1972) suggested that individuals come to know their own attitudes and feelings by inferring them from observations of their own behavior and the circumstances in which this behavior occurs. If people receive an external reward for an activity, they might infer that their motivation for the activity is the reward rather than any intrinsic interest, potentially leading to a decrease in their intrinsic interest in the activity once the reward is removed.

The concept of overjustification effect is related to this discussion. It postulates that when an external incentive is given to a person for an action they already find intrinsically rewarding, they may attribute their action to the incentive, rather than their own interest, thus decreasing their intrinsic motivation (Lepper, Greene & Nisbett, 1973). This suggests a delicate balance is required in using rewards to avoid overjustification.

Despite these concerns, extrinsic motivators are not universally detrimental to intrinsic motivation. Certain types of tasks and individuals may respond better to extrinsic motivators than others. For example, routine, monotonous tasks that are not inherently enjoyable might require external rewards to promote completion and persistence (Condry, 1977). Moreover, when individuals do not perceive an intrinsic value in a task or lack confidence in their abilities, extrinsic rewards can provide the necessary motivation for task engagement and skill development until intrinsic motivation potentially grows (Bandura, 1997).

Furthermore, understanding individual differences is crucial for the application of extrinsic rewards. Some individuals may perceive external rewards as affirmations of their competence and are thus motivated to achieve higher levels of performance, while others may view them as coercive and controlling, which could undermine their intrinsic motivation (Deci et al., 1999). Customization of motivational strategies to align with individual preferences and perceptions can be a more effective approach than a one-size-fits-all model.

Building upon this understanding, it's worth considering the role of feedback in extrinsic motivation. While feedback can function as an extrinsic motivator, when it is aligned with an individual's intrinsic goals and values, it can be particularly powerful. The key is that feedback should be constructive and aimed at helping individuals improve their skills or knowledge, rather than just serving as a mechanism to control behavior (Hattie & Timperley, 2007). When feedback is provided in a way that enhances an individual's mastery of a subject or task, it supports a more autonomous form of motivation, which can interact positively with intrinsic motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000).

In organizational settings, the complexity of extrinsic motivation can be observed in the use of performance appraisals and incentive systems. Goal-setting theory complements the understanding of extrinsic motivation by suggesting that clear, specific, and challenging goals can enhance employee performance (Locke & Latham, 2002). However, if the goals are consistently perceived as too difficult or if the link between performance and rewards is unclear, it can lead to decreased motivation and even counterproductive work behavior (Cawley, Keeping & Levy, 1998).

An important consideration in the use of extrinsic motivators is the time frame of their impact. Short-term increases in motivation may be achieved through incentives, but for long-term sustainability, it's important for the reward system to be perceived as fair and for the rewards to be aligned with individuals' values and needs (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005). This is echoed in self-determination theory, where Deci and Ryan argue that the satisfaction of basic psychological needs, such as autonomy, competence, and relatedness, is essential for long-term motivation and well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000).

The interplay between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation is also evident in educational settings. Gamification, which applies game-design elements in non-game contexts, can use extrinsic motivators like badges and leaderboards to engage students. However, the effects on motivation can be mixed. While gamification can increase participation and immediate engagement, it doesn't always translate to deeper learning or sustained motivation (Dicheva, Dichev, Agre & Angelova, 2015). Teachers and instructional designers therefore face the challenge of embedding such extrinsic elements in ways that do not undermine the intrinsic interest in learning (Sailer, Hense, Mayr & Mandl, 2017).

Similarly, in therapy and health promotion, external rewards can be used to initiate behavior change, such as in weight loss programs or smoking cessation initiatives. Yet, for lasting change, intrinsic motivation is usually necessary, highlighting the importance of moving individuals towards more self-determined forms of motivation over time (Teixeira, Carraa, Markland, Silva & Ryan, 2012).

The implications of these findings for practice are clear: extrinsic motivators can be effective, but their design and implementation must be thoughtful and considerate of the individuals involved. A nuanced approachtaking into account the type of task, individual differences in response to rewards, and the alignment between extrinsic rewards and intrinsic goalscan help maintain or even enhance intrinsic motivation. This delicate balance is the cornerstone of effectively utilizing extrinsic motivation without undermining the inherent drive and satisfaction that comes from engaging with a task for its own sake.

Continuing the exploration of extrinsic motivation, it's crucial to examine the differential effects of various types of extrinsic rewards on motivation and performance. Monetary incentives, for instance, have been a staple in many organizational contexts to drive performance. While financial rewards can be powerful, they can also lead to 'crowding out' of intrinsic motivation, particularly when the tasks are intrinsically interesting or when they require creativity and problem-solving skills (Gneezy, Meier & Rey-Biel, 2011). This can result in a paradoxical decrease in overall motivation if individuals perceive the reward as controlling or undermining their sense of autonomy (Deci, Koestner & Ryan, 1999).

Conversely, non-monetary rewards such as praise, recognition, and opportunities for growth and development can sometimes enhance intrinsic motivation by fulfilling psychological needs for competence and relatedness (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Gagn & Deci, 2005). This suggests that while extrinsic rewards can be effective for repetitive and uninteresting tasks that are not inherently motivating, for more complex and interesting tasks, non-monetary extrinsic rewards that support a person's needs for growth and recognition may be more effective.

Furthermore, the source of extrinsic motivation can also influence its effectiveness. When the source of the motivation is perceived as supportive, providing choice, and acknowledging feelings, it tends to be more in line with intrinsic motivation and is less likely to be experienced as controlling (Deci, Eghrari, Patrick & Leone, 1994). This is why programs that involve participatory decision-making and allow some degree of autonomy over how rewards are earned can result in better outcomes in terms of engagement and internalization of desired behaviors (Gagn & Forest, 2008).

Another aspect to consider is the role of individual differences in responses to extrinsic motivators. Self-Determination Theory posits that individuals differ in their propensity towards autonomy and therefore may react differently to extrinsic rewards (Deci & Ryan, 1985). For example, individuals with a high need for autonomy may find extrinsic rewards less motivating, or even demotivating, compared to individuals with a lower need for autonomy who may respond more positively to such rewards (Vansteenkiste, Lens & Deci, 2006).

Additionally, the cultural context can't be overlooked when discussing the impact of extrinsic motivators. Cultures that place a greater emphasis on collective goals and rewards might view extrinsic motivation ore positively, seeing it as aligned with social harmony and group objectives (Iyengar & DeVoe, 2003). In contrast, cultures with a strong emphasis on individual achievement might respond better to personal rewards and recognition (Triandis, 1994).

As we consider these multiple dimensions of extrinsic motivation, it is evident that no one-size-fits-all approach exists for its application. Individual, contextual, and cultural variables all play crucial roles in determining the effectiveness of extrinsic rewards. Thus, the application of extrinsic incentives must be customized and dynamically-adjusted to fit the needs and perceptions of the individual and their socio-cultural environment. Using extrinsic motivators wisely requires a deep understanding of the individualized interplay between different types of motivation and the delicate balance needed to foster sustained engagement and satisfaction in a given activity.

Conclusion

To optimize motivation, it is critical to consider the nature of the task, the individual's underlying drives, and the potential for internalizing extrinsic motivations into a more self-determined form of motivation. A nuanced approachtaking into account the type of task, individual differences in response to rewards, and the alignment between extrinsic rewards and intrinsic goalscan help maintain or even enhance intrinsic motivation.

References

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American psychologist, 55(1), 68.

Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological bulletin, 125(6), 627.

Gagn, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26(4), 331-362.

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American psychologist, 57(9), 705.

Teixeira, P. J., Carraa, E. V., Markland, D., Silva, M. N., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Exercise, physical activity, and self-determination theory: A systematic review. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9(1), 78.

Cite this Document:
Copy Bibliography Citation

Related Documents

Motivation: Intrinsic Vs Extrinsic Motivation Plays a
Words: 1406 Length: 4 Document Type: Essay

Motivation: Intrinsic vs Extrinsic Motivation plays a critical role in the success of an organization. Employees who are motivated to achieve organizational goals and targets leave a positive impression on the overall performance of that organization. While there are many theories and strategies concerning motivation, the one that is most critical is the debate regarding intrinsic and extrinsic rewards. Motivational theories like Maslow's theory or Vroom's expectancy theory fail to completely

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations at the City Hall
Words: 940 Length: 3 Document Type: Essay

Motivation refers to the driving force that results from desire and an individual's will in life. Motivation has been seen to have roots in the behavioral, physiological, social, and cognitive area. This is rooted in the basic impulse where individual's skills and well-being are utilized for the benefit of the organization. It is the inner drive to act or behave in a certain manner. The innermost conditions of an individual

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation
Words: 1029 Length: 4 Document Type: Essay

The Psychology of Motivation Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being across life's domains Canadian Psychology, 49(1), 14-23. This article examines the appositeness of Self Determination Theory (STD) in optimizing motivation and psychological health for people. The predominant notion explored within this work is that of STD, which is significant for propounding two alternative theories of motivation. The first is that people are motivated by what

Motivation Theory Intrinsic V. Extrinsic
Words: 1006 Length: 4 Document Type: Research Paper

Motivation Theory: Intrinsic v Extrinsic The objective of this study is to examine intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivation. Toward this end this work will examine the literature in this area of study. The work of McCullagh (2005) is reported to state that motivation "can be defined as the intensity and direction of effort." (Wilson, nd, p.1) Wilson reports that motivation is demonstrated in the evidence to promote "learning, performance, enjoyment, and persistent…" (nd,

Sign Up for Unlimited Study Help

Our semester plans gives you unlimited, unrestricted access to our entire library of resources —writing tools, guides, example essays, tutorials, class notes, and more.

Get Started Now