¶ … patterns of physical activity and exercise indicates that there has been an overall trend of decreasing physical activity levels and increasing levels of inactivity among adolescents and adults (Gordon-Larsen, McMurray, & Popkin, 1999; Van Der Horst, Paw, Twisk, & Van Mechelen, 2007). Inactivity and poor diet are responsible a large number of deaths annually and could soon become one of the leading causes of death in developed countries. Research continues to reaffirm that there is a dose response relationship between physical activity and all causes of mortality. There is typically a risk reduction of around 30% for those attaining the recommended levels of at least moderately intense physical activity on most days of the week compared with those who are relatively inactive (Lee & Skerrett, 2001). Regular physical exercise has long been associated with decreased risk of fatal cardiovascular disease (Powell, Thompson, Caspersen, & Kenderick, 1987), obesity and diabetes (Knowler, Barrett-Connor, Fowler, Hamman, Lachin, Walker, & Nathan, 2002), all forms of cancer, (Wannamethee, Shaper, & Walker, 2001), and better psychosocial health (Lawlor & Hopker, 2001). But these studies have documented the benefits of moderate to vigorous exercise in adults on adult health. Childhood physical activity levels (e.g., physical exercise) have traditionally been presumed to influence later levels of activity as an adult (Kuth & Cooper, 1992). Do childhood experiences, particularly childhood and adolescent activity levels, influence later levels of activity and lower the risk factors for early manifestations of the aforementioned illnesses in adults?
Childhood Activity Predicts Adult Activity Levels
In one of the earlier investigations of how childhood activity levels predict adult activity levels Dennison, Straus, Mellits, and Charney (1988) looked at how the physical activity levels of 453 men from 23 to 25 years of age compared with their physical fitness test scores when they were 10 to 11 or 15 to 18 years of age. The findings indicated that physically active adults displayed significantly better childhood physical fitness test scores than did the physically inactive adults. Half of the subjects had physical fitness test scores measured at both childhood and adolescent age levels and this relationship remained robust across all periods of measurement. Other predictors of childhood and adult activity levels included the subjects' reports of being encouraged by their parents to exercise when young, whether or not the subject had participated in organized sports after high school, and spousal encouragement of exercise. These findings suggested that strong social supports for exercise early in development and as an adult may predict patterns of adult activity. Educational level was also a solid predictor of adult activity levels such that more highly educated adults tended to exercise more often or be more physically active than lower educated adults.
One of the criticisms voiced concerning the above study is that childhood physical activity was indirectly measured by the child's score on standardized fitness measures and not by actual measurements or reports of activity levels in children and adolescents. This could present a potential bias in the findings as perhaps not all children scoring high on these fitness measures were actually more active than other children not scoring high and vice versa. Subsequent research has addressed this issue mostly by means utilizing self-report or other report measures of activity in children. For example, Kuh and Cooper (1992) investigated how self-reported childhood activity levels influenced adult activity levels on a stratified sample of 3500 men and women that were followed over period from birth until age 43. Findings indicated that both higher levels of childhood activity and being more practiced or comfortable with sports were associated with greater levels of activity in adults. Raitakan, Porkka, Taimela, Telama, Rasanen, and Vllkari (1994) drew on data from the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. This study began in 1980 with cohorts of randomly sampled boys and girls ages 3, 6, 9, 12, 15, and 18 years-old being examined for the first in a series of repeated measurements. The measures were repeated in three-year increments when the subjects were age 24, 27, 30, 33, 36, and 39 years-old respectively. Physical activity was measured by means of a self-report questionnaire. The findings indicated that a high level of physical activity at ages 9 through age18, especially when it was continuous over these years, significantly predicted a higher level of adult physical activity.
These trends between childhood activity levels and adult activity levels have been consistent in the literature, but are not always replicated (Gordon-Larsen, McMurray, & Popkin, 1999). However, it is well-known that childhood physical activity declines...
Our semester plans gives you unlimited, unrestricted access to our entire library of resources —writing tools, guides, example essays, tutorials, class notes, and more.
Get Started Now