¶ … memory on Learning Disabilities. I believe that there is a strong correlation between the two and that short-term memory is directly affected by Learning Disabilities.
Participants in this first study (Mastropieri, Scruggs, Hamilton, Wolfe, Whedon & Canevaro, 1996) included 29 students identified by their schools as having Learning Disabilities (LD) and were attending seventh- and eighth-grade special education classes in both urban and rural or small-town schools in a Midwestern state. On average, the 10 boys and 19 girls were 14 years 9 months old ( SD = 9 months) and had an average IQ (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, Wechsler, 1974; Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition, Wechsler, 1991) of 87.7 ( SD = 13.0). Average reading grade equivalent, as measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised, Basic Academic Skills Individual Screener, Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement, or Woodcock-Johnson Psycho educational Battery, was 3.5 ( SD = 1.6); average math grade equivalent. The sample included seven African-American students, one Hispanic student, 1 American Indian student, and 20 White, non-Hispanic students. Students had been enrolled in special education, on average, for 5.0 years (SD = 1.4). Twenty-four students received all academic instruction in special education settings, four students were in special education for half of the school day, and one student received special education services for 2 hr/day.
Students were trained and tested individually by one of three (one male, two female) graduate students with teaching certifications in LD. As students entered a quiet area near their regularly assigned classroom, they were stratified by grade level and randomly assigned to one of two conditions.
Because the mean IQ of the experimental group students (85.7) differed somewhat from the mean IQ of control group students (89.5), and because IQ was correlated significantly (p < .01) with recall and explanation scores (r = .45), factual recall and explanation scores were entered into a one-way analysis of covariance, using IQ as the covariate. Analysis was conducted on number of items from each category scored correctly, using means adjusted for the covariate. For factual recall, the adjusted mean for experimental condition students was 68.4% correct, whereas the adjusted mean for control students was 66.8% correct. These scores were not significantly different, F (1, 26) = 0.02, p = .886. For explanation scores, the adjusted mean for experimental condition students was 29.2% correct, whereas the adjusted mean for control condition students was 14.3% correct. These scores were significantly different, F (1, 26) = 5.76, p = .024 (Mastropieri, Scruggs, Hamilton, Wolfe, Whedon & Canevaro, 1996, p. 8).
Further descriptive analysis by Mastropieri, Scruggs, Hamilton, Wolfe, Whedon & Canevaro (1996), conducted for heuristic purposes, and revealed that 71.4% of experimental students and 46.6% of control students scored above 50% correct on the recall measure. On the explanation measure, only one student in each condition scored above 50% correct. However, on that same measure, 42.9% of experimental students and 13.3% of control students scored above 25% correct. Results of this investigation suggest that students who are instructed in actively reasoning through content were able to provide more accurate explanations for that content than were students who were simply encouraged to try to remember the information. This suggests that prompting and training students to use active reasoning resulted in higher levels of comprehension of that information, as measured by their ability to explain underlying relationships. Students did not statistically differ by experimental condition on recall of facts, although a descriptively higher percentage of experimental condition students answered more than half of the recall questions correctly.
Students who were instructed to reason actively through new content information did not remember that information any better statistically than students who were simply instructed to try to remember the information. This result does not replicate the findings of the three previous investigations, in which enhanced factual recall was obtained from active coaching of each item. It was also disappointing to note the relatively low explanation scores. Although experimental students exceeded control students in overall correct explanations, neither group provided what may be considered an acceptable level of explanations for the presented information. That control students would explain little of the information that they had read was expected; however, it was disappointing to note the relatively small increase in explanation scores that resulted from training students to reason independently through expository prose. Results of generalization training have suggested that students with LD may exhibit difficulty with cognitive strategies that require independent insight on the part of students. In the case of mnemonic strategies, students appeared to exhibit difficulty accessing relevant, acoustically similar keywords from their prior knowledge (e.g., "Think of a word that sounds like Taft"). Similar to the results...
Our semester plans gives you unlimited, unrestricted access to our entire library of resources —writing tools, guides, example essays, tutorials, class notes, and more.
Get Started Now