Verified Document

Child's Drawing Ability Drawing Complexity As The Essay

¶ … CHILD'S DRAWING ABILITY Drawing complexity as the complexity or the level of difficulty involved in children's drawing. Drawings from younger children can be less simple with fewer features but as the age of the child progresses the complexity of the drawings increases due to the complex cognitive development.

Drawings are mirror representation of the child's development. Children's drawings have significant roles in the cognitive development of the child. Other roles include training the brain of the child to pay attention and to sustain attention, stimulating individual cells and clusters of cells in the visual cortex for line and shape, practicing and to organizing the shapes and patterns of thought and, through an increasing affinity for marks, to prepare the mind of the child for its determining behavior

Understanding children's cognitive development has implications for many fields, and in particular for education. There exists many possible approaches to the study of cognitive development, assessing a child's drawings can provide a window into their representational world. An example is the ability of a child to depict spatial elements from their environment through an understanding of where an object is located in comparison to others. This is an important aspect of child development as well as an important aspect of geography, geometry, and graphic design. (Missaghi et al. 1991)

There are two types of drawing that children can have, this include; figurative drawing or representational and expressive drawing. Figurative drawing conveys information about figurative aspects of objects in a more or less realistic way. This depends majorly on the age of the child. Expressive drawing has to do with information on mood or the emotions. Any improvement on expressive drawing can be related to the use of content expression and abstract expression

Factors that affects a child's drawing ability

Among factors that affect children's drawing ability include:

1. Level of support and the nature of the design task

2. Child's level of understanding of the drawing.

3. The child's previous experience in drawing

Aim

1. To determine if drawing complexity increases with increase in age of the child

2. To determine the relationship between gender and children's drawing

Hypothesis

1. Drawing complexity increases with increase in age of the child

2. There is no relationship between gender and children's drawing

Method

Participants

A total of 120 children (57 boys and 63 girls) aged between 4 -- 14-years children's drawings were selected for analysis. The selection was done with the consent of the parents. Each of the three age groups comprised a similar number of boys and girls (35 4-6-year-olds, 42 7-11-year-olds, and 37-12-14-year-olds. Before the study began children were given some incentives in form of confectionery, this was done with the sole intention of winning their attention.

Procedure

The children were tested individually in a quiet room. Each child was exposed to two sets of still pictures that were presented on a computer screen and was asked to label each picture. The still pictures were 20 in number. When the participant was done with the first drawings, he or she was given a pencil and a piece of white, standard size paper. The child was given five minutes to draw a picture following the instructions: After each drawing session, the participant was asked to recall the pictures s/he had seen previously.

Results

The drawings were coded by three trained experimenters. The degree of agreement amont the raters (Interrupter reliability) was determined to be 81%. The drawings from the children were analyzed individually. A total of 120 drawings were analyzed (see table 2 for details -- one for each age group, for each type of drawing). Sex and age groups were analyzed separately

SD = 1.21). The two older age groups did not differ significantly in their drawing of essential details. As hypothesized, there is a significantly relationship between drawing complex and the age of the children. Separate chi square tests on the dichotomous variables by sex and age were computed. For the family drawings, results indicated significant sex differences for clothing (?2[1, n = 109] = 6.31, p = .012), the use of stereotyping (?2[1, n = 109] = 13.16, p = .001) and proportionality (?2 [1, n = 109] = 4.81, p = .028 Girls include clothing and stereotyped features in their drawings than boys

Description of expressive techniques

Table 1

Type of expressive technique

Description

Literal

Drawings show facial expressions like happy and smiling face. Personification of non-human topics

Metaphorical

Drawing featuring abstract cues

Both (Literal and metaphorical)

The expressive drawing combines both facial expressions and the content cues

Table 2

Number of children in different age groups age

4-6 years

7-11 years

12-14 years

Plain drawing

30

28

Such parameters like speed, automation and pressure increases with age, therefore there exist a variability with the drawings and the age.
Understanding children's representational development is an essential component for constructing a more complete picture of cognitive development. The representation of knowledge in long-term memory has been clearly seen as an important explanatory component of memory performance and cognitive development (DeLoache, 1987). One important issue that all theories of cognitive development must address is the age differences in how children represent experience.

In general, the results of this study support the hypothesis that with age children's ability to create more complex drawings increases and, in particular, the difference in representational complexity is linked, partially, to an increased working memory capacity as measured by an independent memory recall task (Bensur et al., 1997). The still pictures drawings showed that, consistent with our hypothesis that older children included significantly more essential and inessential details than younger children in their drawings (Bensur et al.,

1997; Golomb, 2004); they were more likely to include additional features and were less likely than younger children to use stick figures.

The current findings of this study also showed significant sex differences among the drawings. Girls were more likely than boys to include additional features in their drawings like add clothes, add stereotyped details for example fingernails and hairstyles to their figures, and draw proportionate human figures. These findings can related to the fact that, on average, girls draw significantly more inessential details than boys, (Callaghan, 2000) Previous studies shows that girls tend to depict more details in their drawings. Together, these results suggest that the girls' drawings may represent their experiences with family relationships. In general, girls tend to value relationships more than boys and they tend to pay more attention to what other same-sex friends wear (Hanline et al., 2007). In general, the depictions may not necessarily reflect a different stage of intellectual development. In addition, there were no age related differences in the number of children who used stereotyped items in their drawing, suggesting that girls may develop a prototype for what it means to be female. This full-scale or the prototype remains rather invariant over time. It may reflect the attempt of females to draw things as they are known rather than as they are actually perceived, also it may be a means for differentiating females from males.

The type of drawings generally made by the different genders is a subject of debate (Troseth et al. 1998) found differences between drawings made by boys and girls aged 5 to 6 years. Marked contrasts were noted in the use of motifs and colors. Girls are fond of drawing people, flowers, butterflies, the sun, houses and buildings, and trees using warm colors more frequently than boys. In contrast, boys like drawing mobile objects such as vehicles, trains, aircrafts and rockets, using cold colors. Girls generally used more colors per drawing as compared to boys.

Summary

In summary children should be given opportunities in all areas to make them know and understand on their own through design or invent. Designedly drawings should become part of the child's drawing.

A gender difference in children's cognitive abilities is an area that needs further research. Whereas some studies have found such differences, others have failed to isolate them. There is no question that additional analyses of the potential gender differences in cognitive development using a large sample with an ample age range are needed and may help to clarify the interaction between gender and age in relation to cognition. This study analyzes gender differences in a sizeable sample of children using the attention, perceptual, language, memory (coding),

The results of this research supports the assumption that gender differences during cognitive development are minimal, appear in only a small number of tests, and account for only a low percentage of the score variance. It is likely, therefore, that certain cultural factors may be responsible for at least some of the gender differences. One limitation of the current study is that the domains used as dependent measures cannot be considered to be underlined by unique cognitive factors as indicated by the factor analyses. Future research should confirm null results of gender differences in cognitive tasks with unique underlying cognitive processes.

Conclusion

The findings of the present study reveal individual differences related to sex, as well as to divergent thinking abilities, in children ability to draw figures that express an idea. As an additional…

Sources used in this document:
References

Bensur, B.J., Eliot, J., and Hegde, L. (1997). Cognitive correlates of complexity of children's drawings. Perceptual and motor skills, 85, 1079 to 1089.

Callaghan, T.C. (2000). Factors affecting children's graphic symbol use in the third year.

Language, similarity, and iconicity. Cognitive Development, 15, 185 -- 214.

Cherney, I. D & Seiwert, C. S & Dickey, T.M. & Flichtbail, J.D. (2006). Children's drawings: a mirror to their minds. Educational psychology, 26(1), 127-142.
Cite this Document:
Copy Bibliography Citation

Related Documents

Children's Drawing Ability and Cognitive Development There
Words: 1720 Length: 6 Document Type: Essay

Children's Drawing Ability and Cognitive Development There is scarcely a refrigerator door in America in homes with children that does not have one or more pictures attached to it with magnets providing proof positive that these young learners are expressing themselves in healthy ways. Over time, these pictorial representations typically increase in complexity and begin to actually resemble the things they are intended to represent, and most parents accept this

Children's Literature to Explore Social
Words: 1280 Length: 4 Document Type: Essay

Because schools matter so greatly in shaping the destiny of each child, they have always been the focus of intense, often unfriendly, attention. Criticisms of the system have always been abundant, and the targets of dissatisfaction have remained virtually the same over generations (13). By properly utilizing children's literature to identify and then to address social issues, more equity can be engendered within a social system in which those from

Growth of a Child From Infancy to
Words: 2514 Length: 7 Document Type: Essay

Growth of a Child from Infancy to Adolescence When a child is born, it is virtually helpless and unable to complete any form of operational tasks. Though a superior being above many creatures, the infant will be able to grow from infancy to adulthood in areas of physical, intellectual, language, emotional, and social development. Every stage of the child's life provides milestones in which will display their growth to full development.

Neanderthal Cultural Complexity
Words: 5339 Length: 15 Document Type: Term Paper

Glimpse into Neanderthal Culture When one thinks of the Humanoid genus Homo Sapiens neanderthalensis (HSN) they picture a very primitive creature, simplistic in nature with few social complexities. However, upon close examination of several Neanderthan archeological sites, one will find the Neanderthal man had all of the necessary elements for the beginning of the formation of modern society. It was once thought that these elements were only present after Neanderthan culture

Overpopulation and Instability: Drawing Connections
Words: 3816 Length: 13 Document Type: Research Proposal

In a study of 50 failed states and political structures, the authors found that in nearly every case (47 of the 50) there was a direct link between out of control population growth and the depletion of specific resources that allow both the country's government and economy the means to create a stable society (Levin, and Renelt, 1992, pp. 954). Overpopulation was but one of a set of variables

Childhood Development Cognitive Behavioral Analysis Paper on
Words: 1466 Length: 4 Document Type: Case Study

Childhood Development Cognitive behavioral analysis paper on child 2 years old Analyzing play situations: Applying Piaget's theories to toddlers The developmental psychologist Jean Piaget, "emphasized the importance of schemas in cognitive development, and described how they were developed or acquired. A schema can be defined as a set of linked mental representations of the world, which we use both to understand and to respond to situations. The assumption is that we store these

Sign Up for Unlimited Study Help

Our semester plans gives you unlimited, unrestricted access to our entire library of resources —writing tools, guides, example essays, tutorials, class notes, and more.

Get Started Now